animal sexual behavior
WordNet
- a living organism characterized by voluntary movement (同)animate being, beast, brute, creature, fauna
- marked by the appetites and passions of the body; "animal instincts"; "carnal knowledge"; "fleshly desire"; "a sensual delight in eating"; "music is the only sensual pleasure without vice" (同)carnal, fleshly, sensual
- the action or reaction of something (as a machine or substance) under specified circumstances; "the behavior of small particles can be studied in experiments" (同)behaviour
- (psychology) the aggregate of the responses or reactions or movements made by an organism in any situation (同)behaviour
- manner of acting or controlling yourself (同)behaviour, conduct, doings
- of or relating to or characterized by sexuality; "sexual orientation"; "sexual distinctions"
- having or involving sex; "sexual reproduction"; "sexual spores"
- (Jungian psychology) the inner self (not the external persona) that is in touch with the unconscious
PrepTutorEJDIC
- (植物に対して)『動物』 / (人間以外の)『動物』,けだもの / (魚・鳥などに対して)哺乳動物(mammal) / けだもの(野獣)のような人 / (比較変化なし)《名詞の前にのみ用いて》動物の,動物性の,動物質の / (知的・精神的に対して)動物的な;肉欲的な
- 性の,男女(雌雄)の / 有性生殖の
Wikipedia preview
出典(authority):フリー百科事典『ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)』「2015/09/13 01:25:14」(JST)
wiki en
Animal sexual behaviour takes many different forms, even within the same species. Common mating or reproductively motivated systems include monogamy, polyandry, polygamy, and promiscuity. Other sexual behaviour may be reproductively motivated (e.g. sex apparently due to duress or coercion and situational sexual behaviour) or non-reproductively motivated (e.g. interspecific sexuality, sexual arousal from objects or places, sex with dead animals, homosexual sexual behaviour, bisexual sexual behaviour, and a range of other behaviours).
When animal sexual behaviour is reproductively motivated, it is often termed mating or copulation; for most non-human mammals, mating and copulation occur at estrus (the most fertile period in the mammalian female's reproductive cycle), which increases the chances of successful impregnation.[1][2] Some animal sexual behaviour involves competition, sometimes fighting, between multiple males. Females often select males for mating only if they appear strong and able to protect themselves. The male that wins a fight may also have the chance to mate with a larger number of females and will therefore pass on his genes to their offspring.[3]
Historically, it was believed that only humans and a small number of other species performed sexual acts other than for reproduction, and that animals' sexuality was instinctive and a simple "stimulus-response" behaviour. A range of species masturbate and may use objects as tools to help them do so.
Contents
- 1 Mating systems
- 1.1 Monogamy
- 1.2 Polygyny
- 1.3 Polyandry
- 1.4 Polygynandry
- 1.5 Polygamy
- 2 Parental investment and reproductive success
- 3 Seasonality
- 3.1 Mammals
- 3.2 Amphibians
- 3.3 Fish
- 4 Motivation
- 4.1 Neurohormones in the mating systems of voles
- 4.2 Oxytocin and rat sexual behaviour
- 4.3 Oxytocin and primate sexual behaviour
- 4.4 Pleasure
- 5 Koinophilia
- 6 Interpretation bias
- 7 Types of sexual behaviour
- 7.1 Reproductive sexual behaviour
- 7.1.1 Cuckoldry
- 7.1.2 Hermaphroditism
- 7.1.3 Sexual cannibalism
- 7.1.4 Sexual coercion
- 7.1.5 Parthenogenesis
- 7.1.6 Unisexuality
- 7.1.7 Others
- 7.2 Non-reproductive sexual behaviour
- 7.2.1 Seahorse
- 7.2.2 Bonobo
- 7.2.3 Dolphin
- 7.2.4 Hyena
- 7.1 Reproductive sexual behaviour
- 8 Mating behaviour
- 8.1 Vertebrates
- 8.1.1 Mammals
- 8.2 Invertebrates
- 8.1 Vertebrates
- 9 Genetic evidence of interspecies sexual activity
- 10 See also
- 11 References
- 12 Bibliography
- 13 Further reading
- 14 External links
Mating systems
In sociobiology and behavioural ecology, the term "mating system" is used to describe the ways in which animal societies are structured in relation to sexual behaviour. The mating system specifies which males mate with which females, and under what circumstances. There are four basic systems:
The four basic mating systems[4]:160–161[5] | ||
---|---|---|
Single female | Multiple females | |
Single male | Monogamy | Polygyny |
Multiple males | Polyandry | Polygynandry |
Monogamy
Monogamy occurs when one male mates with one female exclusively. A monogamous mating system is one in which individuals form long-lasting pairs and cooperate in raising offspring. These pairs may last for a lifetime, such as in pigeons,[6] or it may occasionally change from one mating season to another, such as in emperor penguins.[7] In contrast with tournament species, these pair-bonding species have lower levels of male aggression, competition and little sexual dimorphism. Zoologists and biologists now have evidence that monogamous pairs of animals are not always sexually exclusive. Many animals that form pairs to mate and raise offspring regularly engage in sexual activities with extra-pair partners.[8][9][10][11] This includes previous examples, such as swans. Sometimes, these extra-pair sexual activities lead to offspring. Genetic tests frequently show that some of the offspring raised by a monogamous pair come from the female mating with an extra-pair male partner.[9][12][13][14] These discoveries have led biologists to adopt new ways of talking about monogamy According to Ulrich Reichard (2003):
Social monogamy refers to a male and female's social living arrangement (e.g., shared use of a territory, behaviour indicative of a social pair, and/or proximity between a male and female) without inferring any sexual interactions or reproductive patterns. In humans, social monogamy takes the form of monogamous marriage. Sexual monogamy is defined as an exclusive sexual relationship between a female and a male based on observations of sexual interactions. Finally, the term genetic monogamy is used when DNA analyses can confirm that a female-male pair reproduce exclusively with each other. A combination of terms indicates examples where levels of relationships coincide, e.g., sociosexual and sociogenetic monogamy describe corresponding social and sexual, and social and genetic monogamous relationships, respectively.[15]
Whatever makes a pair of animals socially monogamous does not necessarily make them sexually or genetically monogamous. Social monogamy, sexual monogamy, and genetic monogamy can occur in different combinations.
Social monogamy is relatively rare in the animal kingdom. The actual incidence of social monogamy varies greatly across different branches of the evolutionary tree. Over 90% of avian species are socially monogamous.[10][16] This stands in contrast to mammals. Only 3% of mammalian species are socially monogamous, although up to 15% of primate species are.[10][16] Social monogamy has also been observed in reptiles, fish, and insects.
Sexual monogamy is also rare among animals. Many socially monogamous species engage in extra-pair copulations, making them sexually non-monogamous. For example, while over 90% of birds are socially monogamous, "on average, 30% or more of the baby birds in any nest [are] sired by someone other than the resident male."[17] Patricia Adair Gowaty has estimated that, out of 180 different species of socially monogamous songbirds, only 10% are sexually monogamous.[18]
The incidence of genetic monogamy, determined by DNA fingerprinting, varies widely across species. For a few rare species, the incidence of genetic monogamy is 100%, with all offspring genetically related to the socially monogamous pair. But genetic monogamy is strikingly low in other species. Barash and Lipton note:
The highest known frequency of extra-pair copulations are found among the fairy-wrens, lovely tropical creatures technically known as Malurus splendens and Malurus cyaneus. More than 65% of all fairy-wren chicks are fathered by males outside the supposed breeding group.[16]p. 12
Such low levels of genetic monogamy have surprised biologists and zoologists, forcing them to rethink the role of social monogamy in evolution. They can no longer assume social monogamy determines how genes are distributed in a species. The lower the rates of genetic monogamy among socially monogamous pairs, the less of a role social monogamy plays in determining how genes are distributed among offspring.
Polygyny
Polygyny occurs when one male gets exclusive mating rights with multiple females. In some species, notably those with harem-like structures, only one of a few males in a group of females will mate. Technically, polygyny in sociobiology and zoology is defined as a system in which a male has a relationship with more than one female, but the females are predominantly bonded to a single male. Should the active male be driven out, killed, or otherwise removed from the group, in a number of species the new male will ensure that breeding resources are not wasted on another male's young.[19] The new male may achieve this in many different ways, including:
- competitive infanticide: in lions, hippopotamuses, and some monkeys, the new male will kill the offspring of the previous alpha male to cause their mothers to become receptive to his sexual advances since they are no longer nursing.
- harassment to miscarriage: amongst wild horses and baboons, the male will "systematically harass" pregnant females until they miscarry.
- Pheromone based spontaneous abortion
- in some rodents such as mice, a new male with a different scent will cause females who are pregnant to spontaneously fail to implant recently fertilized eggs. This does not require contact; it is mediated by scent alone. It is known as the Bruce effect.
Von Haartman specifically described the mating behaviour of the European pied flycatcher as successive polygyny.[20] Within this system, the males leave their home territory once their primary female lays her first egg. Males then create a second territory, presumably in order to attract a secondary female to breed. Even when they succeed at acquiring a second mate, the males typically return to the first female to exclusively provide for her and her offspring.[21]
Polygynous mating structures are estimated to occur in up to 90% of mammal species.[22] As polygyny is the most common form of polygamy among vertebrates (including humans, to some extent), it has been studied far more extensively than polyandry or polygynandry.
Polyandry
Polyandry occurs when one female gets exclusive mating rights with multiple males. In some species, such as redlip blennies, both polygyny and polyandry are observed.[23]
The males in some deep sea anglerfishes are much smaller than the females. When they find a female they bite into her skin, releasing an enzyme that digests the skin of their mouth and her body and fusing the pair down to the blood-vessel level. The male then slowly atrophies, losing first his digestive organs, then his brain, heart, and eyes, ending as nothing more than a pair of gonads, which release sperm in response to hormones in the female's bloodstream indicating egg release. This extreme sexual dimorphism ensures that, when the female is ready to spawn, she has a mate immediately available.[24] A single anglerfish female can "mate" with many males in this manner.
Polygynandry
Polygynandry occurs when multiple males mate indiscriminately with multiple females. The numbers of males and females need not be equal, and in vertebrate species studied so far, there are usually fewer males. Two examples of systems in primates are promiscuous mating chimpanzees and bonobos. These species live in social groups consisting of several males and several females. Each female copulates with many males, and vice versa. In bonobos, the amount of promiscuity is particularly striking because bonobos use sex to alleviate social conflict as well as to reproduce. This mutual promiscuity is the approach most commonly used by spawning animals, and is perhaps the "original fish mating system."[4]:161 Common examples are forage fish, such as herrings, which form huge mating shoals in shallow water. The water becomes milky with sperm and the bottom is draped with millions of fertilized eggs.[4]:161
Polygamy
The term polygamy is an umbrella term used to refer generally to non-monogamous matings. As such, polygamous relationships can be polygynous, polyandrous or polygynandrous. In a small number of species, individuals can display either polygamous or monogamous behaviour depending on environmental conditions. An example is the social wasp Apoica flavissima.[citation needed]
A tournament species is one in which "mating tends to be highly polygamous and involves high levels of male-male aggression and competition."[25] Tournament behaviour often correlates with high levels of sexual dimorphism, examples of species including chimpanzees and baboons. Most polygamous species present high levels of tournament behaviour, with a notable exception being bonobos.
Parental investment and reproductive success
Female and male sexual behaviour differ in many species. Often, males are more active in initiating mating, and bear the more conspicuous sexual ornamentation like antlers and colourful plumage. This is a result of anisogamy, where sperm are smaller and much less costly (energetically) to produce than eggs. This difference in physiological cost means that males are more limited by the number of mates they can secure, while females are limited by the quality of genes of her mates, a phenomenon known as Bateman's principle.[26] Many females also have extra reproductive burdens in that parental care often falls mainly, or exclusively, on them. Thus, females are more limited in their potential reproductive success.[27] In species where males take on more of the reproductive costs, such as sea horses and jacanas, the role is reversed, and the females are larger, more aggressive and more brightly coloured than the males.
In hermaphroditic animals, the costs of parental care can be evenly distributed between the sexes, e.g. earthworms. In some species of planarians, sexual behaviour takes the form of penis fencing. In this form of copulation, the individual that first penetrates the other with the penis, forces the other to be female, thus carrying the majority of the cost of reproduction.[28] Post mating, banana slugs will some times gnaw off their partners penis as an act of sperm competition.[29] In the grey slug, the sharing of cost leads to a spectacular display, where the mates suspend themselves high above the ground from a slime thread, ensuring none of them can refrain from taking on the cost of egg-bearer.[30]
Seasonality
Many animal species have specific mating (or breeding) periods e.g. (seasonal breeding) so that offspring are born or hatch at an optimal time. In marine species with limited mobility and external fertilization like corals, sea urchins and clams, the timing of the common spawning is the only externally visible form of sexual behaviour. In areas with continuously high primary production, some species have a series of breeding seasons throughout the year. This is the case with most primates (who are primarily tropical and subtropical animals). Some animals (opportunistic breeders) breed dependent upon other conditions in their environment aside from time of year.
Mammals
Mating seasons are often associated with changes to herd or group structure, and behavioural changes, including territorialism amongst individuals. These may be annual (e.g. wolves), biannual (e.g. dogs) or more frequently (e.g. horses). During these periods, females of most mammalian species are more mentally and physically receptive to sexual advances, a period scientifically described as estrous but commonly described as being "in season" or "in heat". Sexual behaviour may occur outside estrus,[31] and such acts as do occur are not necessarily harmful.[32]
Some mammals (e.g. domestic cats, rabbits and camilidae) are termed "induced ovulators". For these species, the female ovulates due to an external stimulus during, or just prior, to mating, rather than ovulating cyclically or spontaneously. Stimuli causing induced ovulation include the sexual behaviour of coitus, sperm and pheromones. Domestic cats have penile spines. Upon withdrawal of a cat's penis, the spines rake the walls of the female's vagina, which may cause ovulation.[33][34]
Amphibians
For many amphibians, an annual breeding cycle applies, typically regulated by ambient temperature, precipitation, availability of surface water and food supply. This breeding season is accentuated in temperate regions, in boreal climate the breeding season is typically concentrated to a few short days in the spring.
Fish
Like many coral reef dwellers, the clownfish spawn around the time of the full moon in the wild. In a group of clownfish, there is a strict dominance hierarchy. The largest and most aggressive female is found at the top. Only two clownfish, a male and a female, in a group reproduce through external fertilization. Clownfish are sequential hermaphrodites, meaning that they develop into males first, and when they mature, they become females. If the female clownfish is removed from the group, such as by death, one of the largest and most dominant males will become a female. The remaining males will move up a rank in the hierarchy.
Motivation
Various neurohormones stimulate sexual wanting in animals. In general, studies have suggested that dopamine is involved in sexual incentive motivation, oxytocin and melanocortins in sexual attraction, and noradrenaline in sexual arousal.[35] Vasopressin is also involved in the sexual behaviour of some animals.[36]
Neurohormones in the mating systems of voles
The mating system of prairie voles is monogamous; after mating, they form a lifelong bond. In contrast, montane voles have a polygamous mating style. When montane voles mate, they form no attachments and separate after copulation. Studies on the brains of these two species have found that it is two neurohormones and their respective receptors that are responsible for these differences in mating strategies. Male prairie voles release vasopressin after copulation prairie vole and attachment to the female then develops. Female prairie voles release oxytocin after reproducing and an attachment to her partner also develops. Neither male of female montane voles have such a high quantity of oxytocin and vasopressin when they mate. Even when injected with these neurohormones, the mating system does not change. In contrast, if prairie voles are injected with the neurohormones, they may form a lifelong attachment, even if they have not mated. The reason for this is that prairie voles have more oxytocin and vasopressin receptors than do montane voles, and are therefore far more receptive to the two neurohormones. It is not the quantity of the hormone that determines the mating system and bond-formation, rather, it is number of neurohormone receptors.[36]
Oxytocin and rat sexual behaviour
Mother rats experience a postparum estrus which makes them highly motivated to mate. However, they also have a strong motivation to protect their newly born pups. As a consequence, the mother rat solicits males to the nest but simultaneously becomes aggressive towards them to protect her young. If the mother rat is given injections of an oxytocin receptor antagonist, they no longer experience these maternal motivations.[37]
Prolactin influences social bonding in rats.[37]
Oxytocin and primate sexual behaviour
Oxytocin plays a similar role in non-human primates as it does in humans.
Grooming, sex, and cuddling frequencies correlate positively with levels of oxytocin. As the level of oxytocin increases so does sexual motivation. While oxytocin plays a major role in parent child relationships, it is also found to play a role in adult sexual relationships. Its secretion affects the nature of the relationship or if there will even be a relationship at all.[citation needed]
Studies have shown that oxytocin is higher in monkeys in lifelong monogamous relationships compared to monkeys which are single. Furthermore, the oxytocin levels of the couples correlate positively; when the oxytocin secretion of one increases the other one also increases. Higher levels of oxytocin are related to monkeys expressing more behaviours such as cuddling, grooming and sex, while lower levels of oxytocin reduce motivation for these activities.[citation needed]
Research on oxytocin's role in the animal brain suggests that it plays less of a role in behaviours of love and affection than previously believed. "When oxytocin was first discovered in 1909, it was thought mostly to influence a mother’s labor contractions and milk let-down. Then, in the 1990s, research with prairie voles found that giving them a dose of oxytocin resulted in the formation of a bond with their future mate (Azar, 40)." Oxytocin has since been treated by the media as the sole player in the "love and mating game" in mammals. This view, however, is proving to be false as, "most hormones don’t influence behaviour directly. Rather, they affect thinking and emotions in variable ways (Azar, 40)." There is much more involved in sexual behaviour in the mammalian animal than oxytocin and vasopressin can explain.[38][39][40][41][42][43][44]
Pleasure
It is often assumed that animals do not have sex for pleasure, or alternatively that humans, pigs (and perhaps dolphins and one or two species of primate) are the only species that do. This is sometimes stated as "animals mate only for reproduction". This view is considered a misconception by some scholars.[45] Jonathan Balcombe argues that the prevalence of non-reproductive sexual behavior in certain species suggests that sexual stimulation is pleasurable. He also points to the presence of the clitoris in some female mammals, and evidence for female orgasm in primates.[46] On the other hand, it is impossible to know the subjective feelings of animals,[35] and the notion that non-human animals experience emotions similar to humans is a contentious subject.[47][48][49][50]
A 2006 Danish Animal Ethics Council report[51] which examined current knowledge of animal sexuality in the context of legal queries concerning sexual acts by humans, has the following comments, primarily related to domestically common animals:
Even though the evolution-related purpose of mating can be said to be reproduction, it is not actually the creating of offspring which originally causes them to mate. It is probable that they mate because they are motivated for the actual copulation, and because this is connected with a positive experience. It is therefore reasonable to assume that there is some form of pleasure or satisfaction connected with the act. This assumption is confirmed by the behaviour of males, who in the case of many species are prepared to work to get access to female animals, especially if the female animal is in oestrus, and males who for breeding purposes are used to having sperm collected become very eager, when the equipment they associate with the collection is taken out.[52]
There is nothing in female mammals' anatomy or physiology that contradicts that stimulation of the sexual organs and mating is able to be a positive experience. For instance, the clitoris acts in the same way as with women, and scientific studies have shown that the success of reproduction is improved by stimulation of clitoris on (among other species) cows and mares in connection with insemination, because it improves the transportation of the sperm due to contractions of the inner genitalia. This probably also concerns female animals of other animal species, and contractions in the inner genitals are seen e.g. also during orgasm for women. It is therefore reasonable to assume that sexual intercourse may be linked with a positive experience for female animals.[53]
Koinophilia
Koinophilia is the love of the "normal" or phenotypically common (from the Greek, koinos, meaning "the usual" or "common").[54] The term was introduced to scientific literature in 1990, and refers to the tendency of animals seeking a mate to prefer that mate not to have any unusual, peculiar or deviant features.[54] Similarly, animals preferentially choose mates with low fluctuating asymmetry. [55] However, animal sexual ornaments can evolve through runaway selection, which is driven by (usually female) selection for non-standard traits.[56]
Interpretation bias
The field of study of sexuality in non-human species was a long-standing taboo.[57] In the past, researchers sometimes failed to observe, mis-categorizing and mis-described sexual behaviour which did not meet their preconceptions. In earlier periods, bias tended to support what would now be described as conservative sexual mores. An example of overlooking behaviour relates to descriptions of giraffe mating:
When nine out of ten pairings occur between males, "[e]very male that sniffed a female was reported as sex, while anal intercourse with orgasm between males was only [categorized as] 'revolving around' dominance, competition or greetings."[57]
In the 21st century, liberal social or sexual views are often projected upon animal subjects of research. Popular discussions of bonobos are a frequently cited example. Current research frequently expresses views such as that of the Natural History Museum at the University of Oslo, which in 2006 held an exhibition on animal sexuality:
"Many researchers have described homosexuality as something altogether different from sex. They must realise that animals can have sex with who they will, when they will and without consideration to a researcher's ethical principles."[57]
Other animal activities may be misinterpreted due to the frequency and context in which animals perform the behaviour. For example, domestic ruminants display behaviours such as mounting and head-butting. This often occurs when the animals are establishing dominance relationships and are not necessarily sexually-motivated. Careful analysis must be made to interpret what animal motivations are being expressed by those behaviours.[58]
Types of sexual behaviour
Reproductive sexual behaviour
Cuckoldry
Alternate male strategies which allow small males to engage in cuckoldry can develop in species such as fish where spawning is dominated by large and aggressive males. Cuckoldry is a variant of polyandry, and can occur with sneak spawners. A sneak spawner is a male that rushes in to join the spawning rush of a spawning pair.[59] A spawning rush occurs when a fish makes a burst of speed, usually on a near vertical incline, releasing gametes at the apex, followed by a rapid return to the lake or sea floor or fish aggregation.[60] Sneaking males do not take part in courtship. In salmon and trout, for example, jack males are common. These are small silvery males that migrate upstream along with the standard, large, hook-nosed males and that spawn by sneaking into redds to release sperm simultaneously with a mated pair. This behaviour is an evolutionarily stable strategy for reproduction, because it is favoured by natural selection just like the "standard" strategy of large males.[61]
Hermaphroditism
Hermaphroditism occurs when a given individual in a species possesses both male and female reproductive organs, or can alternate between possessing first one, and then the other. Hermaphroditism is common in invertebrates but rare in vertebrates. It can be contrasted with gonochorism, where each individual in a species is either male or female, and remains that way throughout their lives. Most fish are gonochorists, but hermaphroditism is known to occur in 14 families of teleost fishes.[62]
Usually hermaphrodites are sequential, meaning they can switch sex, usually from female to male (protogyny). This can happen if a dominant male is removed from a group of females. The largest female in the harem can switch sex over a few days and replace the dominant male.[62] This is found amongst coral reef fishes such as groupers, parrotfishes and wrasses. It is less common for a male to switch to a female (protandry).[4]:162 As an example, most wrasses are protogynous hermaphrodites within a haremic mating system.[63][64] Hermaphroditism allows for complex mating systems. Wrasses exhibit three different mating systems: polygynous, lek-like, and promiscuous mating systems.[65]
Sexual cannibalism
Sexual cannibalism is a behaviour in which a female animal kills and consumes the male before, during, or after copulation. Sexual cannibalism confers fitness advantages to both the male and female.[citation needed] Sexual cannibalism is common among insects, arachnids[66] and amphipods.[66] There is also evidence of sexual cannibalism in gastropods and copepods.[67]
Sexual coercion
Sex in a forceful or apparently coercive context has been documented in a variety of species. In some herbivorous herd species, or species where males and females are very different in size, the male dominates sexually by force and size.[citation needed]
Some species of birds appear to combine sexual intercourse with apparent violent assault; these include ducks,[68][69] and geese.[citation needed] Female white-fronted bee-eaters are subjected to forced copulations. When females emerge from their nest burrows, males sometimes force them to the ground and mate with them. Such forced copulations are made preferentially on females who are laying and who may therefore lay eggs fertilised by the male.[70]
In the Acilius genus of water beetles, an "evolutionary arms race" between the two sexes means that there is no courtship system for these beetles. Their mating behaviours include males holding females underwater till exhausted, and allowing only occasional access to the surface to breathe for up to six hours (to prevent them breeding with other males), and females which have a variety of body shapings (to prevent males from gaining a grip).[71]
It has been reported that young male elephants in South Africa sexually coerced and killed rhinoceroses.[72] This interpretation of the elephants' behaviour was disputed by one of the original study's authors, who said there was "nothing sexual about these attacks".[73]
Parthenogenesis
Parthenogenesis is a form of asexual reproduction in which growth and development of embryos occur without fertilization. Technically, parthenogenesis it is not a behaviour, however, sexual behaviours may be involved.
Whip-tailed lizard females have the ability to reproduce through parthenogenesis and as such males are rare and sexual breeding non-standard. Females engage in "pseudocopulation"[74] to stimulate ovulation, with their behaviour following their hormonal cycles; during low levels of estrogen, these (female) lizards engage in "masculine" sexual roles. Those animals with currently high estrogen levels assume "feminine" sexual roles. Lizards that perform the courtship ritual have greater fecundity than those kept in isolation due to an increase in hormones triggered by the sexual behaviours. So, even though asexual whiptail lizards populations lack males, sexual stimuli still increase reproductive success. From an evolutionary standpoint these females are passing their full genetic code to all of their offspring rather than the 50% of genes that would be passed in sexual reproduction.[citation needed]
It is rare to find true parthenogenesis in fishes, where females produce female offspring with no input from males. All-female species include the Texas silverside, Menidia clarkhubbsi[75] as well as a complex of Mexican mollies.[4]:162
Parthenogenesis has been recorded in 70 vertebrate species[76] including hammerhead sharks,[77] blacktip sharks,[78] amphibians[79][80] and crayfish.[81][82]
Unisexuality
Unisexuality occurs when a species is all-male or all-female. Unisexuality occurs in some fish species, and can take complex forms. Squalius alburnoides, a minnow found in several river basins in Portugal and Spain, appears to be an all-male species. The existence of this species illustrates the potential complexity of mating systems in fish. The species originated as a hybrid between two species, and is diploid, but not hermaphroditic. It can have triploid and tetraploid forms, including all-female forms that reproduce mainly through hybridogenesis.[83]
Others
- Interbreeding: Hybrid offspring can result from the mating of two organisms of distinct but closely related parent species, although the resulting offspring is not always fertile. According to Alfred Kinsey, genetic studies on wild animal populations have shown a "large number" of inter-species hybrids.[84]
- Prostitution: There are reports that animals occasionally engage in prostitution. A small number of pair-bonded females within a group of penguins took nesting material (stones) after copulating with a non-partner male. The researcher stated "I was watching opportunistically, so I can't give an exact figure of how common it really is."[85] It has been reported that "bartering of meat for sex ... forms part of the social fabric of a troop of wild chimps living in the Tai National Park in the Cote d'Ivoire."[86]
- Pavlovian conditioning: The sexualization of objects or locations is recognized in the animal breeding world. For example, male animals may become sexually aroused upon visiting a location where they have been allowed to have sex before, or upon seeing a stimulus previously associated with sexual activity such as an artificial vagina.[52] Sexual preferences for certain cues can be artificially induced in rats by pairing scents or objects with their early sexual experiences.[87] The primary motivation of this behaviour is Pavlovian conditioning, and the association is due to a conditioned response (or association) formed with a distinctive "reward".[87]
- Viewing images: A study using four adult male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) showed that male rhesus macaques will give up a highly valued item, juice, to see images of the faces or perineum of high-status females.[88] Encouraging captive pandas to mate is problematic. Showing young male pandas "panda pornography" is credited with a recent population boom among pandas in captivity in China. One researcher attributed the success to the sounds on the recordings.[89]
- Copulatory wounding and Traumatic Insemination: Injury to a partner’s genital tract during mating occurs in at least 40 taxa, ranging from fruit flies to humans. However, it often goes unnoticed due to its cryptic nature and because of internal wounds not visible outside.[90]
Non-reproductive sexual behaviour
There is a range of behaviours that animals perform which appear to be sexually motivated but which can not result in reproduction. These include -
- Masturbation: Some species, both male and female, masturbate, both when partners are available and otherwise.[91][92]
- Oral sex: Several species engage in both autofellatio and oral sex. This has been documented in brown bears,[93] Tibetan macaques,[94] wolves,[95] goats, primates, hyenas,[96] bats,[97] cape ground squirrels[98] and sheep. In the greater short-nosed fruit bat, copulation by males is dorsoventral and the females lick the shaft or the base of the male's penis, but not the glans which has already penetrated the vagina. While the females do this, the penis is not withdrawn and research has shown a positive relationship between length of the time that the penis is licked and the duration of copulation. Post copulation genital grooming has also been observed.[99]
- Homosexuality: Same-sex sexual behaviour occurs in a range of species, especially in social species, particularly in marine birds and mammals, monkeys, and the great apes. As of 1999, the scientific literature contained reports of homosexual behavior in at least 471 wild species.[100] Organisers of the Against Nature? exhibit stated that "homosexuality has been observed among 1,500 species, and that in 500 of those it is well documented."[101]
- Genital-genital rubbing: This is sexual activity in which one animal rubs his or her genitals against the genitals of another animal. This is stated to be the "bonobo's most typical sexual pattern, undocumented in any other primate".[103][104]
- Inter-species mating: Some animals opportunistically mate with individuals of another species.[105]
- Sex involving juveniles: Male stoats (Mustela erminea) will sometimes mate with infant females of their species.[106] This is a natural part of their reproductive biology – they have a delayed gestation period, so these females give birth the following year when they are fully grown. Juvenile male chimpanzees have been recorded mounting and copulating with immature chimps. Infants in bonobo societies are often involved in sexual behaviour.[107]
- Necrophilia: This describes when an animal engages in a sexual act with a dead animal. It has been observed in mammals, birds, reptiles and frogs.[108]
- Bisexuality: This describes when an animal shows sexual behaviour toward both males and females.
Seahorse
Seahorses, once considered to be monogamous species with pairs mating for life, were described in a 2007 study as "promiscuous, flighty, and more than a little bit gay".[109] Scientists at 15 aquaria studied 90 seahorses of three species. Of 3,168 sexual encounters, 37% were same-sex acts. Flirting was common (up to 25 potential partners a day of both sexes); only one species (the British spiny seahorse) included faithful representatives, and for these 5 of 17 were faithful, 12 were not. Bisexual behaviour was widespread and considered "both a great surprise and a shock", with big-bellied seahorses of both sexes not showing partner preference. 1,986 contacts were male-female, 836 were female-female and 346 were male-male.[109]
Bonobo
The bonobo, which has a matriarchal society, is a fully bisexual species — both males and females engage in sexual behaviour with the same and the opposite sex, with females being particularly noted for engaging in sexual behaviour with each other and at up to 75% of sexual activity being nonreproductive. Primatologist Frans de Waal believes that bonobos use sexual activity to resolve conflict between individuals.[110] Sexual activity occurs between almost all ages and sexes of bonobo societies.[111]
Dolphin
Male bottlenose dolphins have been observed working in pairs to follow or restrict the movement of a female for weeks at a time, waiting for her to become sexually receptive. The same pairs have also been observed engaging in intense sexual play with each other. Janet Mann, a professor of biology and psychology at Georgetown University, argues[112] that the common same-sex behaviour among male dolphin calves is about bond formation and benefits the species evolutionarily. They cite studies that have shown the dolphins later in life are bisexual and the male bonds forged from homosexuality work for protection as well as locating females with which to reproduce. In 1991, an English man was prosecuted for allegedly having sexual contact with a dolphin.[113] The man was found not guilty after it was revealed at trial that the dolphin was known to tow bathers through the water by hooking his penis around them.[113]
Hyena
The female spotted hyena has a unique urinary-genital system, closely resembling the penis of the male, called a pseudo-penis. The family structure is matriarchal and dominance relationships with strong sexual elements are routinely observed between related females. They are notable for using visible sexual arousal as a sign of submission but not dominance in males as well as females (females have a sizable erectile clitoris).[114] It is speculated that to facilitate this, their sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems may be partially reversed in respect to their reproductive organs.[115]
Mating behaviour
Vertebrates
Mammals
Mammals mate by vaginal copulation. To achieve this, the male usually mounts the female from behind. The female may exhibit lordosis in which she arches her back ventrally to facilitate entry of the penis. Other than humans, only bonobos mate in a face-to-face position.[116][better source needed] Some camelids mate in a lying-down position.[117] In most ruminant species, a single pelvic thrust occurs during copulation.[118]
Invertebrates
Invertebrates are often hermaphrodites. Some hermaphroditic land snails begin mating with an elaborate tactile courting ritual. The two snails circle around each other for up to six hours, touching with their tentacles, and biting lips and the area of the genital pore, which shows some preliminary signs of the eversion of the penis. As the snails approach mating, hydraulic pressure builds up in the blood sinus surrounding an organ housing a sharpened dart. The dart is made of calcium carbonate or chitin, and is called a love dart. Each snail manoeuvres to get its genital pore in the best position, close to the other snail's body. Then, when the body of one snail touches the other snail's genital pore, it triggers the firing of the love dart.[119] After the snails have fired their darts, they copulate and exchange sperm as a separate part of the mating progression. The love darts are covered with a mucus that contains a hormone-like substance that facilitates the survival of the sperm.[120][121]
Penis fencing is a mating behaviour engaged in by certain species of flatworm, such as Pseudobiceros bedfordi. Species which engage in the practice are hermaphroditic, possessing both eggs and sperm-producing testes.[122] The species "fence" using two-headed dagger-like penises which are pointed, and white in color. One organism inseminates the other. The sperm is absorbed through pores in the skin, causing fertilization.
Corals can be both gonochoristic (unisexual) and hermaphroditic, each of which can reproduce sexually and asexually. Reproduction also allows corals to settle new areas. Corals predominantly reproduce sexually. 25% of hermatypic corals (stony corals) form single sex (gonochoristic) colonies, while the rest are hermaphroditic.[123] About 75% of all hermatypic corals "broadcast spawn" by releasing gametes—eggs and sperm—into the water to spread offspring. The gametes fuse during fertilization to form a microscopic larva called a planula, typically pink and elliptical in shape.[124] Synchronous spawning is very typical on the coral reef and often, even when multiple species are present, all corals spawn on the same night. This synchrony is essential so that male and female gametes can meet. Corals must rely on environmental cues, varying from species to species, to determine the proper time to release gametes into the water. The cues involve lunar changes, sunset time, and possibly chemical signalling.[123] Synchronous spawning may form hybrids and is perhaps involved in coral speciation.[125]
Butterflies spend much time searching for mates. When the male spots a mate, he will fly closer and closer and release pheromones. He then performs a special courtship dance to attract the female. If the female appreciates the dancing she may join him. Then they join their bodies together end to end at their abdomens. Here, the male passes the sperm to the female's egg-laying tube which will soon be fertilized by the sperm.[126] The male often dies shortly after mating.
Many animals make plugs of mucus to seal the female's orifice after mating. Normally such plugs are secreted by the male, to block subsequent partners. In spiders the female can assist the process.[127] Spider sex is unusual in that males transfer their sperm to the female on small limbs called pedipalps. They use these to pick their sperm up from their genitals and insert it into the female's sexual orifice, rather than copulating directly.[127] On the 14 occasions a sexual plug was made, the female produced it without assistance from the male. On ten of these occasions the male's pedipalps then seemed to get stuck while he was transferring the sperm (which is rarely the case in other species of spider), and he had great difficulty freeing himself. In two of those ten instances, he was eaten as a result.[127]
Genetic evidence of interspecies sexual activity
Research into human evolution confirms that, in some cases, interspecies sexual activity may have been responsible for the evolution of new species (speciation). Analysis of animal genes found evidence that after humans had diverged from other apes, interspecies mating nonetheless occurred regularly enough to change certain genes in the new gene pool.[128] Researchers found that the X chromosomes of humans and chimps may have diverged around 1.2 million years after the other chromosomes. One possible explanation is that modern humans emerged from a hybrid of human and chimp populations.[129] A 2012 study questioned this explanation, concluding that "there is no strong reason to involve complicated factors in explaining the autosomal data".[130]
See also
Sexuality portal | |
Animals portal |
- Pre-copulatory isolation mechanisms in animals
- Biology and sexual orientation
- Green Porno, a series of short films about animal mating, enacted by humans, airing on the Sundance Channel
- List of animals displaying homosexual behaviour
- r/K selection theory
References
- ^ Kent, Michael (2000). Advanced biology. Oxford University Press. pp. 250–253. ISBN 978-0-19-914195-1.
- ^ Thorpe, Showick and Thorpe, Edgar (2009). General Studies Manual. Pearson Education India. p. page 17 (xvii). ISBN 9788131721339. CS1 maint: Extra text (link)
- ^ Wickler, Wolfgang; Lorenz; Konrad and Kacher, Hermann (1974). "The sexual code : the social behaviour of animals and me".
- ^ a b c d e Moyle PB and Cech JJ (2004) Fishes, An Introduction to Ichthyology. 5th Ed, Benjamin Cummings. ISBN 978-0-13-100847-2
- ^ Berglund A (1997) "Mating systems and sex allocation" Pages 237–265 in JJ Godon, ed. Behavioural ecology of teleost fishes. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-850503-5.
- ^ "Pigeon". Encyclopædia Britannica. Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 26 August 2014.
- ^ Lily Whiteman (13 February 2013). "Animal Attraction: The Many Forms of Monogamy in the Animal Kingdom". National Science Foundation. National Science Foundation. Retrieved 26 August 2014.
- ^ Ågren, G.; Zhou, Q.; Zhong, W. (1989). "Ecology and social behaviour of Mongolian gerbils, Meriones unguiculatus, at Xilinhot, Inner Mongolia, China". Animal Behaviour 37: 11–27. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(89)90002-X.
- ^ a b Birkhead, T.R. & Møller, A.P.; Møller (1995). "Extra-pair copulations and extra-pair paternity in birds". Animal Behaviour 49 (3): 843–848. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(95)80217-7.
- ^ a b c Reichard, U.H. (2002). "Monogamy—A variable relationship" (PDF). Max Planck Research 3: 62–67. Retrieved 24 April 2013. [dead link]
- ^ Westneat, D. F.; Stewart, I. R. K. (2003). "EXTRA-PAIRPATERNITY INBIRDS: Causes, Correlates, and Conflict". Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 34: 365–396. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132439.
- ^ Birkhead, T.R. & Møller, A.P. (1996) "Monogamy and sperm competition in birds". In J. M. Black (Ed.), Partnerships in Birds: The Study of Monogamy, pp. 323–343, Oxford: Oxford University Press ISBN 0-19-854860-5.
- ^ Owens, I. P. F.; Hartley, I. R. (1998). "Sexual dimorphism in birds: Why are there so many different forms of dimorphism?". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 265 (1394): 397–407. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0308. JSTOR 50849. PMC 1688905.
- ^ Solomon, N. G.; Keane, B.; Knoch, L. R.; Hogan, P. J. (2004). "Multiple paternity in socially monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster)". Canadian Journal of Zoology 82 (10): 1667–1671. doi:10.1139/z04-142.
- ^ Reichard, U.H. (2003). Monogamy: Past and present. In U.H. Reichard and C. Boesch (Eds.), Monogamy: Mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans, and other mammals, pp. 3–25, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, ISBN 0521819733.
- ^ a b c Barash, D.P. & Lipton, J.E. (2001). The Myth of Monogamy. New York, NY: W.H. Freeman and Company, ISBN 0805071369.
- ^ Angier, Natalie (21 August 1990). "Mating for Life? It's Not for the Birds of the Bees". The New York Times.
- ^ Morell, V. (1998). "EVOLUTION OF SEX:A New Look at Monogamy". Science 281 (5385): 1982–1983. doi:10.1126/science.281.5385.1982. PMID 9767050.
- ^ This section and examples taken from Robert Sapolsky (1998) Why Zebras Don't Get Ulcers, W.H. Freeman and Co., ISBN 0-7167-3210-6, pp. 140–141.
- ^ Haartman, L. V. (1951). "Successive Polygamy". Behaviour 3: 256–273. doi:10.1163/156853951X00296.
- ^ Silverin, B. (1979). "Effects of long-acting testosterone administration on testes in free living pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca". Endokrinologie 74 (2): 141–146. PMID 583410.
- ^ Aloise King, Edith D.; Banks, Peter B.; Brooks, Robert C. (2011). "Sexual conflict in mammals: consequences for mating systems and life history". Mammal Review (January 2013) 43 (1): 47–58. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2907.2011.00200.x.
- ^ Marraro, CH; Nursall JR (1983). "The reproductive periodicity and behaviour of Ophioblennius atlanticus at Barbados". J Zool 61 (2): 317–325. doi:10.1139/z83-042.
- ^ Theodore W. Pietsch. "Precocious sexual parasitism in the deep sea ceratioid anglerfish, Cryptopsaras couesi Gill". Retrieved 31 July 2008.
- ^ Robert Sapolsky (2005). "Biology and Human Behavior: The Neurological Origins of Individuality, 2nd edition". The Teaching Company. Retrieved 2010-11-10.
- ^ Bateman, A.J. (1948), "Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila", Heredity 2 (Pt. 3): 349–368, doi:10.1038/hdy.1948.21, PMID 18103134
- ^ Trivers, R.L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871-1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine. ISBN 0-435-62157-2.
- ^ Hermaphrodites duel for manhood, Science News Online. Accessed 14 March 2009.
- ^ Miller, Brooke L. W (2007). Sexual conflict and partner manipulation in the banana slug, Ariolimax dolichophallus. SANTA CRUZ: UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. Retrieved 13 February 2015.
- ^ Leonard, J. L. (5 May 2006). "Sexual selection: lessons from hermaphrodite mating systems". Integrative and Comparative Biology 46 (4): 349–367. doi:10.1093/icb/icj041. Retrieved 11 February 2015.
- ^ For example, masturbation, trial mounting, and other behaviours are regularly seen in male animals out of season
- ^ 2006 Danish Animal Ethics Council report: "The mucous membrane in the female animal’s vagina and the animal’s behaviour is under influence of its rut cycle. That means that the animal is physically and mentally more ready for sexual activities at some times than at others. But this does not mean that sexual activity will lead to injuries, fear or suffering, if it happens outside the rut period."
(Danish: "Slimhinden i hundyrets vagina og dyrets adfærd er under indflydelse af dets brunstcyklus. Det betyder, at dyret er fysisk og mentalt mere parat til seksuelle aktiviteter på nogle tidspunkter end på andre. Men dette er ikke ensbetydende med, at den seksuelle aktivitet vil være forbundet med skader, angst og lidelse, hvis den foregår udenfor brunstperioden.") Report November 2006 (PDF)[dead link] - ^ Virginia Douglass Hayssen; Ari Van Tienhoven (1993). Asdell's Patterns of Mammalian Reproduction: A Compendium of Species-specific Data. Cornell University Press. ISBN 978-0-8014-1753-5. Retrieved 27 September 2013.
- ^ Aronson, L. R.; Cooper, M. L. (1967). "Penile spines of the domestic cat: their endocrine-behavior relations." (PDF). Anatomical Record 157 (1): 71–78. doi:10.1002/ar.1091570111. PMID 6030760.
- ^ a b Georgiadis, J. R., Kringelbach, M. L., & Pfaus, J. G. (2012). "Sex for fun: a synthesis of human and animal neurobiology". Nature Reviews Urology 9 (9): 486–498. doi:10.1038/nrurol.2012.151.
- ^ a b Bear, Mark F. (2007). Neuroscience, exploring the brain. Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. pp. 544–545. ISBN 0781760038.
- ^ a b Kennett, J.E.and McKee, D.T. (2012). "Oxytocin: An emerging regulator of prolactin secretion in the female rat.". Journal of Neuroendocrinology 24 (3): 403–412. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02263.x. PMC 3288386. PMID 22129099.
- ^ Lambert, K. (2011). The Lab Rat Chronicles. New York: penguin group. pp. 151–172.
- ^ Azar, B. (March 2011). "Oxytocin's other side". Science Watch 42 (3): 40.
- ^ Aubert, Y.; Gustison, M. L.; Gardner, L. A.; Bohl, M. A.; Lange, J. R.; Allers, K. A.; Sommer, B.; Datson, N. A.; Abbott, D. H. (2012). "Flibanserin and 8-OH-DPAT Implicate Serotonin in Association between Female Marmoset Monkey Sexual Behavior and Changes in Pair-Bond Quality". The Journal of Sexual Medicine 9 (3): 694–707. doi:10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02616.x. PMID 22304661.
- ^ Gil, M.; Bhatt, R.; Picotte, K. B.; Hull, E. M. (2011). "Oxytocin in the medial preoptic area facilitates male sexual behavior in the rat". Hormones and Behavior 59 (4): 435–443. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.12.012. PMC 3081415. PMID 21195714.
- ^ Scott, Graham (2004). Essential Animal Behavior. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 166–197. ISBN 0632057998.
- ^ Agrati, D.; Fernández-Guasti, A.; Ferreño, M.; Ferreira, A. (2011). "Coexpression of sexual behavior and maternal aggression: The ambivalence of sexually active mother rats toward male intruders". Behavioral Neuroscience 125 (3): 446–451. doi:10.1037/a0023085. PMID 21517149.
- ^ McHenry, J. A.; Bell, G. A.; Parrish, B. P.; Hull, E. M. (2012). "Dopamine D1 receptors and phosphorylation of dopamine- and cyclic AMP-regulated phosphoprotein-32 in the medial preoptic area are involved in experience-induced enhancement of male sexual behavior in rats". Behavioral Neuroscience 126 (4): 523–529. doi:10.1037/a0028707. PMC 3409344. PMID 22708956.
- ^ Jeffrey Moussaieff Masson (21 October 2009). When Elephants Weep: The Emotional Lives of Animals. Random House Publishing Group. ISBN 978-0-307-57420-6. Retrieved 28 May 2013.
- ^ Balcombe, J. (2009). "Animal pleasure and its moral significance". Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118 (3): 212. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.012.
- ^ Dawkins, M. (2000). "Animal minds and animal emotions". American Zoologist 40 (6): 883–888. doi:10.1668/0003-1569(2000)040[0883:amaae]2.0.co;2.
- ^ Panksepp, J. (1982). "Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3): 407–422. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00012759.
- ^ "Emotions help animals to make choices (press release)". University of Bristol. 2010. Retrieved October 26, 2013.
- ^ Jacky Turner and Joyce D'Silva, ed. (2006). Animals, Ethics and Trade: The Challenge of Animal Sentience. Earthscan. ISBN 9781844072545. Retrieved October 26, 2013.
- ^ Det Dyreetiske Råd (2006). "Udtalelse om menneskers seksuelle omgang med dyr" (PDF). Justitsministeriet. Council members included two academics, two farmers/smallholders, and two veterinary surgeons, as well as a third veterinary surgeon acting as secretary.
- ^ a b Det Dyreetiske Råd (2006). "Udtalelse om menneskers seksuelle omgang med dyr" (PDF). Justitsministeriet.
Selv om det evolutionsmæssige formål med at parre sig kan siges at være reproduktion, er det ikke selve det, at dyrene får afkom, der i første omgang får dem til at parre sig. Det er til gengæld sandsynligt, at de parrer sig, fordi de er motiverede for selve parringsakten, og at denne er forbundet med en positiv oplevelse. Det er derfor rimeligt at antage, at der er en eller anden form for behag eller tilfredsstillelse forbundet med akten. Denne antagelse bekræftes af adfærden hos handyr, der for mange arters vedkommende er parate til at arbejde for at få adgang til hundyr, især hvis hundyret er i brunst, og handyr der i avlsøjemed er vant til at få tappet sæd – de viser stor ivrighed, når det udstyr, de forbinder med sædopsamlingen, tages frem.
- ^ Det Dyreetiske Råd (2006). "Udtalelse om menneskers seksuelle omgang med dyr" (PDF). Justitsministeriet.
Der er intet ved hunpattedyrenes anatomi eller fysiologi, der modsiger, at stimulation af kønsorganerne og parring skulle kunne være en positiv oplevelse – fx fungerer klitoris på samme måde som hos kvinder. Videnskabelige undersøgelser har vist, at reproduktionssuccesen forbedres ved stimulation af klitoris på bl.a. køer og hopper i forbindelse med insemination, fordi det forbedrer sædtransporten pga. sammentrækninger af de indre kønsdele. Dette gælder sandsynligvis også hundyr af andre dyrearter, og sammentrækninger i de indre kønsdele ses fx også under orgasme hos kvinder. Det er derfor rimeligt at antage, at det seksuelle samvær kan være forbundet med en positiv oplevelse for hundyrene.
- ^ a b Koeslag, J.H. (1990). "Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour". Journal of Theoretical Biology 144: 15–35. doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80297-8.
- ^ Moller, A.P. and Pomiankowski, A, (1993). "Fluctuating asymmetry and sexual selection". Genetica 89: 267–279. doi:10.1007/bf02424520.
- ^ Dawkins, Richard (1986). The Blind Watchmaker. Longman, London. Published in Penguin Books 1988, 1991, and 2006. Chapter 8, Explosions and Spirals.
- ^ a b c "1,500 animal species practice homosexuality". News-medical.net. 23 October 2006. Retrieved 19 February 2007. [unreliable source?]
- ^ Katz, L. S.; McDonald, T. J. (1992). "Sexual behavior of farm animals". Theriogenology 38 (2): 239–253. doi:10.1016/0093-691X(92)90233-H. PMID 16727133.
- ^ Streak spawning Fishbase Glossary. Retrieved 11 February 2011.
- ^ Spawning rush Fishbase Glossary. Retrieved 11 February 2011.
- ^ Gross MR (1984) "Sunfish, salmon, and the evolution of alternative reproductive strategies and tactics in fishes". Pages 55–75 in GW Potts and RJ Wottoon, eds. Fish reproduction: Strategies and tactics. Academic Press.
- ^ a b Shapiro DY (1984) "Sex reversal and sociodemographics processes in coral reef fishes" Pages 103–116 in GW Potts and RK Wootoon, eds., Fish reproduction: Strategies and tactics, Academic Press.
- ^ Robertson, D.R.; R.R. Warner. "Sexual patterns in the labroid fishes of the Western Caribbean II: the parrotfishes (Scaridae)". Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology 255: 1–26. doi:10.5479/si.00810282.255.
- ^ Kazancioglu, E.; S.H. Alonzo (August 2010). "A comparative analysis of sex change in Labridae supports the size advantage hypothesis". Evolution 64 (8): 2254–2264. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01016.x. PMID 20394662.
- ^ Colin, P.L.; L. J. Bell. "Aspects of the spawning of labrid and scarid fishes (Pisces, Labroidei) at Enewetak Atoll, Marshall Islands with notes on other families (corrected reprint.)". Environmental Biology of Fishes 33 (3): 330–345. doi:10.1007/BF00005881.
- ^ a b Polis, G.A. The evolution and dynamics of intraspecific +4193 predation. Annual Reviews Ecological Systems 51, 225-251 (1981).
- ^ Bilde, T., Tuni, C., Elsayed, R., Pekár, S. & Toft, S. Death feigning in the face of sexual cannibalism. Biology letters 2, 23-5 (2006).
- ^ Bailey, R.O.; Seymour, N. R. and Stewart, G.R. (1978). "Rape behaviour in blue-winged teal" (PDF). Auk 95: 188–90. doi:10.2307/4085514. JSTOR 4085514.
- ^ Barash, D. P. (1977). "Sociobiology of Rape in Mallards (Anas platyrhynchos): Responses of the Mated Male". Science 197 (4305): 788–789. doi:10.1126/science.197.4305.788. PMID 17790773.
- ^ Emlen, S. T.; Wrege, P. H. (2010). "Forced Copulations and Intra-specific Parasitism: Two Costs of Social Living in the White-fronted Bee-eater". Ethology 71: 2–29. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00566.x.
- ^ "Not tonight, not ever. I’ve got a headache. Don’t come near me", The Times, 25 June 2007 p. 25[dead link]
- ^ Siebert, Charles. (2006-10-08) ''An Elephant Crackup?'', Charles Siebert, New York Times Magazine, October 8, 2006. Nytimes.com. Retrieved on 2011-12-22.
- ^ "Have elephants begun raping rhinos in the wild?". The Straight Dope. 25 July 2008. Retrieved 5 March 2013.
- ^ Hiskey, D. "New Mexico whiptail lizards are all females". Retrieved February 13, 2015.
- ^ Echelle AA, Echelle AF, Crozier CD (1983). "Evolution of an all-female fish, Menidia clarkhubbsi (Atherinidae)". Evolution 37 (4): 772–784. doi:10.2307/2407918.
- ^ Harmon, K. (2010). "No sex needed: All-female lizard species cross their chromosomes to make babies.". Scientific American. Retrieved February 13, 2015.
- ^ "Captive shark had 'virgin birth'". BBC News. 2007-05-23. Retrieved 23 December 2008.
- ^ "'Virgin birth' for aquarium shark". Metro.co.uk. 2008-10-10. Retrieved 2008-10-10.
- ^ Halliday, Tim R.; Adler, Kraig, eds. (1986). Reptiles & Amphibians. Torstar Books. p. 101. ISBN 0-920269-81-8.
- ^ Walker, Brian (2010-11-11). "Scientists discover unknown lizard species at lunch buffet". CNN. Retrieved 2010-11-11.
- ^ Scholtz, Gerhard; Braband, Anke; Tolley, Laura; Reimann, André; Mittmann, Beate; Lukhaup, Chris; Steuerwald, Frank; Vogt, GüNter (2003). "Ecology: Parthenogenesis in an outsider crayfish". Nature 421 (6925): 806. Bibcode:2003Natur.421..806S. doi:10.1038/421806a. PMID 12594502.
- ^ Martin, Peer; Kohlmann, Klaus; Scholtz, Gerhard (2007). "The parthenogenetic Marmorkrebs (marbled crayfish) produces genetically uniform offspring". Naturwissenschaften 94 (10): 843–6. Bibcode:2007NW.....94..843M. doi:10.1007/s00114-007-0260-0. PMID 17541537.
- ^ Alves MJ, Collarea-Pereira MJ, Dowling TE, Coelho MM (2002). "The genetics of maintenance of an all-male lineage in the Squalius alburnoides complex". J. Fish. Biol 60: 649–662. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2002.tb01691.x.
- ^ Kinsey, Alfred (1953). Sexual Behavior in the Human Female. W.B. Saunders Company. p. 504.
- ^ "Penguins are turning to prostitution". BBC. 26 February 1998. Retrieved 12 June 2008.
- ^ Connor, Steve (8 April 2009). "Sex for meat – how chimps seduce their mates". The Independent (London).
- ^ a b Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., Coria-Avila, G. A., Gelez, H., Afonso, V. M., Ismail, N., & Parada, M. (2012). "Who, what, where, when (and maybe even why)? How the experience of sexual reward connects sexual desire, preference, and performance". Archives of Sexual Behavior 41 (1): 31–62. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9935-5.
- ^ Deaner M. O., Khera A. V. and Platt M. L. (2005). "Monkeys pay per view: adaptive valuation of social images by rhesus macaques" (PDF). Current Biology 15 (6): 543–548. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.044. PMID 15797023.
- ^ Gray, Denis D. (27 November 2006) Porn sparks panda baby boom in China: Research — and blue movies — attributed to record-high birth rate in 2006. Associated Press (via MSNBC). Retrieved on 22 December 2011.
- ^ Klaus Reinhardt, Nils Anthes, and Rolanda Lange (2015) Copulatory Wounding and Traumatic Insemination" Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015; 7: a017582
- ^ Watson, P. F. (1978). Artificial breeding of non-domestic animals: (the proceedings of a symposium held at the Zoological Society of London on 7 and 8 September 1977). Academic Press for the Zoological Society of London. ISBN 978-0-12-613343-1. Retrieved 9 February 2013.
- ^ Balcombe, Jonathan P. (2011). The Exultant Ark: A Pictorial Tour of Animal Pleasure. University of California Press. pp. 89–. ISBN 978-0-520-26024-5.
- ^ These Bears Are Having Lots Of Oral Sex, And Scientists Think They Know Why (The Huffington Post) By: Grenoble, Ryan.
- ^ Hideshi Ogawa (2006). Wily Monkeys: Social Intelligence of Tibetan Macaques. Kyoto University Press. pp. 4–. ISBN 978-1-920901-97-4.
- ^ Fox, M. W. (1972). "The Social Significance of Genital Licking in the Wolf, Canis lupus". Journal of Mammalogy 53: 637–640. doi:10.2307/1379064. JSTOR 1379064.
- ^ G. R. Pafumi (15 January 2010). Is Our Vision of God Obsolete?. Xlibris Corporation. ISBN 978-1-4500-0396-4.
- ^ Tan, M.; Jones, G.; Zhu, G.; Ye, J.; Hong, T.; Zhou, S.; Zhang, S.; Zhang, L. (2009). Hosken, David, ed. "Fellatio by Fruit Bats Prolongs Copulation Time". PLoS ONE 4 (10): e7595. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007595. PMC 2762080. PMID 19862320.
- ^ Waterman, J. M. (2010). Briffa, Mark, ed. "The Adaptive Function of Masturbation in a Promiscuous African Ground Squirrel". PLoS ONE 5 (9): e13060. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013060. PMC 2946931. PMID 20927404.
- ^ Tan, Min; Gareth Jones; Guangjian Zhu; Jianping Ye; Tiyu Hong; Shanyi Zhou; Shuyi Zhang; Libiao Zhang (28 October 2009). Hosken, David, ed. "Fellatio by Fruit Bats Prolongs Copulation Time". PLoS ONE 4 (10): e7595. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.7595T. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007595. PMC 2762080. PMID 19862320. Retrieved 28 October 2009.
- ^ Bagemihl, Bruce (1999). Biological Exuberance: Animal Homosexuality and Natural Diversity. St. Martin's Press. p. 673.
- ^ "Oslo gay animal show draws crowds". BBC News. 19 October 2006. Archived from the original on 29 October 2006. Retrieved 19 October 2006.
- ^ Sazima, I. (2015). "Corpse bride irresistible: a dead female tegu lizard (Salvator merianae) courted by males for two days at an urban park in South-eastern Brazil". Herpetology Notes 8: 15–18.
- ^ de Waal FB (1995). "Bonobo sex and society". Sci Am. 272 (3): 82–8. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0395-82. PMID 7871411.
Perhaps the bonobo's most typical sexual pattern, undocumented in any other primate, is genito-genital rubbing (or GG rubbing) between adult females. One female facing another clings with arms and legs to a partner that, standing on both hands and feet, lifts her off the ground
- ^ Paoli, T.; Palagi, E.; Tacconi, G.; Tarli, S. B. (2006). "Perineal swelling, intermenstrual cycle, and female sexual behavior in bonobos (Pan paniscus)". American Journal of Primatology 68 (4): 333–347. doi:10.1002/ajp.20228. PMID 16534808.
- ^ Walker, Matt (2 May 2008) Science/Nature 'Sex pest' seal attacks penguin. BBC News. Retrieved on 15 February 2011.
- ^ Doncarlos, Michael W., Petersen, Jay S., Tilson, Ronald L.; Petersen; Tilson (1986). "Captive biology of an asocial mustelid; Mustela erminea". Zoo Biology 5 (4): 363–370. doi:10.1002/zoo.1430050407.
- ^ Dawkins, Richard (2004). "Chimpanzees". The Ancestor's Tale. Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 1-155-16265-X.
- ^ de Mattos Brito, L. B., Joventino, I. R., Ribeiro, S. C., & Cascon, P. (2012). "Necrophiliac behavior in the "cururu" toad, Rhinella jimi Steuvax, 2002, (Anura, Bufonidae) from Northeastern Brazil" (PDF). North-Western Journal of Zoology 8 (2): 365.
- ^ a b Simon De Bruxelles Promiscuous and bisexual — the 'faithful' seahorse has a secret sex life. Timesonline.co.uk. 31 January 2007. Retrieved on 22 December 2011.
- ^ Homosexual Activity Among Animals Stirs Debate, National Geographic (22 July 2004) Retrieved 6 November 2007
- ^ Zihlman, A. L.; Hunter, W. S. (1972). "A biomechanical interpretation of the pelvis of Australopithecus". Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology 18 (1): 1–19. doi:10.1159/000155465. PMID 4658666.
- ^ Central Park Zoo's gay penguins ignite debate. Sfgate.com (7 February 2004). Retrieved on 22 December 2011.
- ^ a b Unwin, Brian (22 January 2008). "'Tougher laws' to protect friendly dolphins". London: The Telegraph.
- ^ East, Marion L.; Hofer, Heribert and Wickler, Wolfgang (1993). "The erect 'penis' is a flag of submission in a female-dominated society: greetings in Serengeti spotted hyenas". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 33 (6): 355–370. doi:10.1007/BF00170251.
- ^ Sapolsky (1998), Why Zebras Don't Get Ulcers, W.H. Freeman and Co., ISBN 0-7167-3210-6, pp. 127–129.
- ^ Fortey, I. (2008). "The 15 most bizarre animal mating rituals". Retrieved February 18, 2015.
- ^ "Bactrian & Dromedary Camels". Factsheets. San Diego Zoo Global Library. March 2009. Retrieved 4 December 2012.
- ^ Donald M. Broom; Andrew Ferguson Fraser (1 January 2007). Domestic Animal Behaviour and Welfare. CABI. pp. 156–. ISBN 978-1-84593-287-9.
- ^ Rogers, David; Chase, Ronald (2001). "Dart receipt promotes sperm storage in the garden snail Helix aspersa". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50 (2): 122–7. doi:10.1007/s002650100345.
- ^ Chase, R. (2007). "The function of dart shooting in helicid snails". American Malacological Bulletin 23: 183–189. doi:10.4003/0740-2783-23.1.183.
- ^ Chase, R.; Blanchard, K. C. (2006). "The snail's love-dart delivers mucus to increase paternity". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences 273 (1593): 1471–1475. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3474.
- ^ Newman, Leslie. "Fighting to mate: flatworm penis fencing". PBS.
- ^ a b Veron, J.E.N. (2000). Corals of the World. Vol 3 (3rd ed.). Australia: Australian Institute of Marine Sciences and CRR Qld Pty Ltd. ISBN 0-642-32236-8.
- ^ Barnes, R. and; Hughes, R. (1999). An Introduction to Marine Ecology (3rd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell Science, Inc. pp. 117–141. ISBN 0-86542-834-4.
- ^ Hatta, M., Fukami, H., Wang, W., Omori, M., Shimoike, K., Hayashibara, T., Ina, Y., Sugiyama, T. (1999). "Reproductive and genetic evidence for a reticulate evolutionary theory of mass spawning corals" (PDF). Molecular Biology and Evolution 16 (11): 1607–1613. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a026073. PMID 10555292.
- ^ "Chan Lee Peng, August 2008". Scienceray.com. Retrieved 5 March 2013.
- ^ a b c Sexual Appetite and Animal behaviour – Sex and the single spider 400 (8742). The Economist Intelligence Unit. 14 July 2011. ISSN 0013-0613.
- ^ Patterson, Nick; Daniel J. Richter; Sante Gnerre; Eric S. Lander; David Reich (June 29, 2006). "Genetic evidence for complex speciation of humans and chimpanzees". Nature 441 (7097): 1103–1108. Bibcode:2006Natur.441.1103P. doi:10.1038/nature04789. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 16710306.
- ^ Wade, Nicholas (18 May 2006) Two Splits Between Human and Chimp Lines Suggested, New York Times.
- ^ Yamamichi, M; Gojobori J; Innan H. (Jan 2012). "An autosomal analysis gives no genetic evidence for complex speciation of humans and chimpanzees". Mol Biol Evol 29 (1): 145–56. doi:10.1093/molbev/msr172. PMID 21903679.
Bibliography
- Bagemihl, Bruce (1999). Biological Exuberance: Animal Homosexuality and Natural Diversity. St. Martin's Press. ISBN 0-312-19239-8.
- Schaller, G.B. (1972). The Serengeti Lion. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0226736601.
Further reading
- Ronald D. Nadler (1980) Reproductive physiology and behaviour of gorillas., in R. V. Short and Barbara J. Weir (eds) The Great Apes of Africa, Cambridge, 1980. Journals of Reproduction and Fertility Ltd.
- Richard Estes (1991). The Behavior Guide to African Mammals: Including Hoofed Mammals, Carnivores, Primates. University of California Press. ISBN 978-0-520-08085-0.
- John Vandenbergh (28 August 1983). Pheromones and Reproduction in Mammals. Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-323-15651-6.
External links
- Media related to Animal sex at Wikimedia Commons
- Media related to Interspecies sex at Wikimedia Commons
- Media related to Mammal sex at Wikimedia Commons
- Media related to Lepidoptera sex at Wikimedia Commons
- Gay flamingos celebrate fifth anniversary with their children
- Gay penguins resist split attempts
- National Geographic
- San Francisco Zoo has run a "sex tour" covering animal sexuality, on Valentine's Day
- A wild, and gay, kingdom World Science
- Is it relevant to look at the animal kingdom to determine if human same-sex behaviour is "natural"?
|
|
|
UpToDate Contents
全文を閲覧するには購読必要です。 To read the full text you will need to subscribe.
- 1. 卵巣の性索間質腫瘍の概要 overview of sex cord stromal tumors of the ovary
- 2. 外陰部異常を有する幼児の評価 evaluation of the infant with ambiguous genitalia
- 3. 思春期の性 adolescent sexuality
- 4. 卵巣の性索間質腫瘍:顆粒膜細胞腫瘍およびセルトリ・ライディック細胞腫 sex cord stromal tumors of the ovary tumors with granulosa and sertoli leydig elements
- 5. 着床前の男女生み分け技術 techniques for preimplantation sex selection
English Journal
- Wnt Signaling in Skin Development, Homeostasis, and Disease.
- Lim X, Nusse R.SourceDepartment of Developmental Biology, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California 94305.
- Cold Spring Harbor perspectives in biology.Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol.2012 Dec 3. pii: cshperspect.a008029v1. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a008029. [Epub ahead of print]
- The skin and its appendages constitute the largest organ of the body. Its stratified epithelia offer protection from environmental stresses such as dehydration, irradiation, mechanical trauma, and pathogenic infection, whereas its appendages, like hair and sebaceous glands, help regulate body temper
- PMID 23209129
- Adolescent gain in positive valence of a socially relevant stimulus: engagement of the mesocorticolimbic reward circuitry.
- Bell MR, De Lorme KC, Figueira RJ, Kashy DA, Sisk CL.SourceNeuroscience Program, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA.
- The European journal of neuroscience.Eur J Neurosci.2012 Nov 22. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12058. [Epub ahead of print]
- A successful transition from childhood to adulthood requires adolescent maturation of social information processing. The neurobiological underpinnings of this maturational process remain elusive. This research employed the male Syrian hamster as a tractable animal model for investigating the neural
- PMID 23173754
Japanese Journal
- Effects of methyl eugenol and raspberry ketone/cue lure on the sexual behavior of Bactrocera species (Diptera: Tephritidae)
- Shelly Todd
- Applied Entomology and Zoology 45(3), 349-361, 2010
- … The purpose of this review is to summarize research on the potential role of ME and RK/CL—as pheromone precursors or components—in the sexual communication of Bactrocera species. … The topics covered include i) male feeding behavior on ME or RK/CL, ii) the fate of ingested lures, iii) female and male attraction to lure-fed males, and iv) mating success of lure-fed males. …
- NAID 130000327175
- マウス由来の人工膵臓を用いた糖尿病雌ラットの高血糖および性機能不全の改善
- HASHIMOTO Haruo,MORITANI Naoki,TERADA Misao,KROMKHUN Pudcharaporn,FUNGFUAUG Wirasak,NAKADA Tomoaki,YOKOSUKA Makoto,SAITO Toru R.
- Experimental Animals 59(4), 515-519, 2010
- … We investigated the effects of a bioartificial endocrine pancreas (Bio-AEP) produced by mouse β cells on sexual dysfunction of streptozotocin (STZ)-induced diabetic female rats. … Lordosis and proceptive sexual behavior of female rats were observed. … The results showed that after the Bio-AEP transplant blood glucose recovered from 380–450 mg/dl induced by streptozotocin to 140–230 mg/dl and suppressed lordosis and proceptive behavior also recovered. …
- NAID 130000298612
- 高齢および若齢雄ラットの交尾行動における心拍数の変化
- TERADA Misao,WATO Seiko,KROMKHUN Pacharaporn,HASHIMOTO Haruo,TANIGUCHI Kazumi,SAKURAI Fujiro,YOKOSUKA Makoto,NAGY Gyorgy G.,SAITO Toru R.
- Experimental Animals 58(4), 427-430, 2009
- … As members of Western societies age, sexual function of older (elderly) individuals becomes an important issue, particularly for men. … Specifically, copulatory behavior increases circulatory load, which may be related to reports of cardiac sudden death following ejaculation. … These results suggest that the circulatory load on the aged rat heart is greater than that on a young rat heart during copulatory behavior, especially at ejaculation. …
- NAID 130000122251
Related Links
- For human sexual practices, see Human sexual behavior Animal sexual behavior takes many different forms, even within the same species. Researchers have observed monogamy, promiscuity, sex between species, sexual arousal ...
- Sexual Behavior I. ANIMAL SEXUAL BEHAVIORCharles H. Phoenix BIBLIOGRAPHY II. SOCIAL ASPECTSSamuel Z. Klausner BIBLIOGRAPHY III. SEXUAL DEVIATION: PSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTSJohn Money BIBLIOGRAPHY ...
Related Pictures
★リンクテーブル★
リンク元 | 「動物性行動」「animal sex behavior」 |
関連記事 | 「behavior」「sexual behavior」「animal」「sexual」 |
「動物性行動」
「animal sex behavior」
「behavior」
- n.
「sexual behavior」
「animal」
- n.
- adj.
- 動物性の
「sexual」
- adj.
- 性的な
- 関
- sexually