touch

出典: meddic

  • n.
  • v.
palpatepalpationpalpatorytactiletactile sensationtactile sense

linux

touch [OPTION]... FILE...
 -a                     change only the access time
 -c, --no-create        do not create any files
 -d, --date=STRING      parse STRING and use it instead of current time
 -f                     (ignored)
 -m                     change only the modification time
 -r, --reference=FILE   use this file's times instead of current time
 -t STAMP               use [[CC]YY]MMDDhhmm[.ss] instead of current time
 --time=WORD            change the specified time:
                          WORD is access, atime, or use: equivalent to -a
                          WORD is modify or mtime: equivalent to -m
     --help     display this help and exit
     --version  output version information and exit
  • 注意:過去の時間は設定できません。
-t 201003221800 *


WordNet   license wordnet

「perceive via the tactile sense; "Helen Keller felt the physical world by touching people and objects around her"」

WordNet   license wordnet

「a slight but appreciable amount; "this dish could use a touch of garlic"」
hint, tinge, mite, pinch, jot, speck, soupcon

WordNet   license wordnet

「a slight attack of illness; "he has a touch of rheumatism"」
spot

WordNet   license wordnet

「a distinguishing style; "this room needs a woman''s touch"」
signature

WordNet   license wordnet

「the act of putting two things together with no space between them; "at his touch the room filled with lights"」
touching

WordNet   license wordnet

「the feel of mechanical action; "this piano has a wonderful touch"」

WordNet   license wordnet

「the faculty by which external objects or forces are perceived through contact with the body (especially the hands); "only sight and touch enable us to locate objects in the space around us"」
sense of touch, skin senses, touch modality, cutaneous senses

WordNet   license wordnet

「the sensation produced by pressure receptors in the skin; "she likes the touch of silk on her skin"; "the surface had a greasy feeling"」
touch sensation, tactual sensation, tactile sensation, feeling

WordNet   license wordnet

「the event of something coming in contact with the body; "he longed for the touch of her hand"; "the cooling touch of the night air"」
touching

WordNet   license wordnet

「a suggestion of some quality; "there was a touch of sarcasm in his tone"; "he detected a ghost of a smile on her face"」
trace, ghost

WordNet   license wordnet

「deftness in handling matters; "he has a master''s touch"」

WordNet   license wordnet

「the act of soliciting money (as a gift or loan); "he watched the beggar trying to make a touch"」

WordNet   license wordnet

「be in direct physical contact with; make contact; "The two buildings touch"; "Their hands touched"; "The wire must not contact the metal cover"; "The surfaces contact at this point"」
adjoin, meet, contact

WordNet   license wordnet

「tamper with; "Don''t touch my CDs!"」
disturb

WordNet   license wordnet

「affect emotionally; "A stirring movie"; "I was touched by your kind letter of sympathy"」
stir

WordNet   license wordnet

「cause to be in brief contact with; "He touched his toes to the horse''s flanks"」

WordNet   license wordnet

「comprehend; "He could not touch the meaning of the poem"」

WordNet   license wordnet

「deal with; usually used with a form of negation; "I wouldn''t touch her with a ten-foot pole"; "The local Mafia won''t touch gambling"」

WordNet   license wordnet

「make physical contact with, come in contact with; "Touch the stone for good luck"; "She never touched her husband"」

PrepTutorEJDIC   license prepejdic

「〈人が〉(手・指などで)…‘を'『さわる』,‘に'触れる《+名+with+名》 / (…に)…‘を'触れさせる,あてがう《+名+to+名》 / 〈物が〉…‘に'触れている,‘と'接触している / …‘を'『感動させる』,の感情を動かす / 《文》〈事が〉…‘に'『影響する』,関係する,かかわる(concern) / 《通例否定文で》〈飲食物〉‘に'手をつける;〈道具など〉‘を'使う / 《しばしば受動態で》(かすかに)…‘に'色合いをつける / (軽く)…‘を'害する,いためる / 《通例否定文で》…‘に'匹敵する,‘と'肩を並べる / 〈船などが〉…‘に'立ち寄る,寄港する / 《俗》《+名+for+名》(金銭などを)〈人に〉‘に'せびる,借りる / さわる,触れる,接触している / (港などに)〈船などが〉立ち寄る,寄港する《+at+名》 / 〈U〉『触覚』,触感 / 〈C〉《単数形で》(物の)感触,手ざわり / 〈C〉『さわる(触れる)こと』,さわられること / 〈U〉《しばしばa ~》(…の)『気味,微候』,気配《+of+名》 / 〈C〉少量,少しばかりの(の…)《+of+名》 / 〈C〉(作品仕上げの)手入れ,一筆 / 〈U〉《しばしばa ~》(作家などの)筆法,作風 / 〈U〉《しばしばa ~》(ピアノ・タイプライターなどの)指運び,たたき方,タッチ」

Wikipedia preview

出典(authority):フリー百科事典『ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)』「2014/09/30 11:33:50」(JST)

wiki ja

[Wiki ja表示]

wiki en

[Wiki en表示]

UpToDate Contents

全文を閲覧するには購読必要です。 To read the full text you will need to subscribe.

英文文献

  • Evaluation of the safety and usability of touch gestures in operating in-vehicle information systems with visual occlusion.
  • Kim H1, Song H2.Author information 1Dept. of Mechanical System Design Engineering, Seoul National University of Science & Technology, 138 Gongneung-gill, Nowon-gu, Seoul 139-743, Republic of Korea. Electronic address: huhnkim@seoultech.ac.kr.2Dept. of Mechanical System Design Engineering, Seoul National University of Science & Technology, 138 Gongneung-gill, Nowon-gu, Seoul 139-743, Republic of Korea.AbstractNowadays, many automobile manufacturers are interested in applying the touch gestures that are used in smart phones to operate their in-vehicle information systems (IVISs). In this study, an experiment was performed to verify the applicability of touch gestures in the operation of IVISs from the viewpoints of both driving safety and usability. In the experiment, two devices were used: one was the Apple iPad, with which various touch gestures such as flicking, panning, and pinching were enabled; the other was the SK EnNavi, which only allowed tapping touch gestures. The participants performed the touch operations using the two devices under visually occluded situations, which is a well-known technique for estimating load of visual attention while driving. In scrolling through a list, the flicking gestures required more time than the tapping gestures. Interestingly, both the flicking and simple tapping gestures required slightly higher visual attention. In moving a map, the average time taken per operation and the visual attention load required for the panning gestures did not differ from those of the simple tapping gestures that are used in existing car navigation systems. In zooming in/out of a map, the average time taken per pinching gesture was similar to that of the tapping gesture but required higher visual attention. Moreover, pinching gestures at a display angle of 75° required that the participants severely bend their wrists. Because the display angles of many car navigation systems tends to be more than 75°, pinching gestures can cause severe fatigue on users' wrists. Furthermore, contrary to participants' evaluation of other gestures, several participants answered that the pinching gesture was not necessary when operating IVISs. It was found that the panning gesture is the only touch gesture that can be used without negative consequences when operating IVISs while driving. The flicking gesture is likely to be used if the screen moving speed is slower or if the car is in heavy traffic. However, the pinching gesture is not an appropriate method of operating IVISs while driving in the various scenarios examined in this study.
  • Applied ergonomics.Appl Ergon.2014 May;45(3):789-98. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2013.10.013. Epub 2013 Nov 11.
  • Nowadays, many automobile manufacturers are interested in applying the touch gestures that are used in smart phones to operate their in-vehicle information systems (IVISs). In this study, an experiment was performed to verify the applicability of touch gestures in the operation of IVISs from the vie
  • PMID 24231034
  • The effect of touch-key size on the usability of In-Vehicle Information Systems and driving safety during simulated driving.
  • Kim H1, Kwon S2, Heo J3, Lee H4, Chung MK5.Author information 1Dept. of Industrial and Management Engineering, Pohang University of Science and Technology (POSTECH), San 31, Hyoja-dong, Nam-gu, Pohang 790-784, South Korea. Electronic address: gimigimi@postech.ac.kr.2Dept. of Industrial and Management Engineering, Pohang University of Science and Technology (POSTECH), San 31, Hyoja-dong, Nam-gu, Pohang 790-784, South Korea. Electronic address: samdolee@postech.ac.kr.3Dept. of Industrial and Management Engineering, Pohang University of Science and Technology (POSTECH), San 31, Hyoja-dong, Nam-gu, Pohang 790-784, South Korea. Electronic address: nzlovpos@gmail.com.4Dept. of Industrial and Management Engineering, Pohang University of Science and Technology (POSTECH), San 31, Hyoja-dong, Nam-gu, Pohang 790-784, South Korea. Electronic address: bluestar@postech.ac.kr.5Dept. of Industrial and Management Engineering, Pohang University of Science and Technology (POSTECH), San 31, Hyoja-dong, Nam-gu, Pohang 790-784, South Korea. Electronic address: mkc@postech.ac.kr.AbstractInvestigating the effect of touch-key size on usability of In-Vehicle Information Systems (IVISs) is one of the most important research issues since it is closely related to safety issues besides its usability. This study investigated the effects of the touch-key size of IVISs with respect to safety issues (the standard deviation of lane position, the speed variation, the total glance time, the mean glance time, the mean time between glances, and the mean number of glances) and the usability of IVISs (the task completion time, error rate, subjective preference, and NASA-TLX) through a driving simulation. A total of 30 drivers participated in the task of entering 5-digit numbers with various touch-key sizes while performing simulated driving. The size of the touch-key was 7.5 mm, 12.5 mm, 17.5 mm, 22.5 mm and 27.5 mm, and the speed of driving was set to 0 km/h (stationary state), 50 km/h and 100 km/h. As a result, both the driving safety and the usability of the IVISs increased as the touch-key size increased up to a certain size (17.5 mm in this study), at which they reached asymptotes. We performed Fitts' law analysis of our data, and this revealed that the data from the dual task experiment did not follow Fitts' law.
  • Applied ergonomics.Appl Ergon.2014 May;45(3):379-88. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2013.05.006. Epub 2013 Jun 4.
  • Investigating the effect of touch-key size on usability of In-Vehicle Information Systems (IVISs) is one of the most important research issues since it is closely related to safety issues besides its usability. This study investigated the effects of the touch-key size of IVISs with respect to safety
  • PMID 23759791
  • Categorical perception of tactile distance.
  • Knight Fle C1, Longo MR2, Bremner AJ3.Author information 1Sensorimotor Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom. Electronic address: f.knight@gold.ac.uk.2Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, United Kingdom.3Sensorimotor Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom.AbstractThe tactile surface forms a continuous sheet covering the body. And yet, the perceived distance between two touches varies across stimulation sites. Perceived tactile distance is larger when stimuli cross over the wrist, compared to when both fall on either the hand or the forearm. This effect could reflect a categorical distortion of tactile space across body-part boundaries (in which stimuli crossing the wrist boundary are perceptually elongated) or may simply reflect a localised increased in acuity surrounding anatomical landmarks (in which stimuli near the wrist are perceptually elongated). We tested these two interpretations across two experiments, by comparing a well-documented bias to perceive mediolateral tactile distances across the forearm/hand as larger than proximodistal ones along the forearm/hand at three different sites (hand, wrist, and forearm). According to the 'categorical' interpretation, tactile distances should be elongated selectively in the proximodistal axis thus reducing the anisotropy. According to the 'localised acuity' interpretation, distances will be perceptually elongated in the vicinity of the wrist regardless of orientation, leading to increased overall size without affecting anisotropy. Consistent with the categorical account, we found a reduction in the magnitude of anisotropy at the wrist, with no evidence of a corresponding localised increase in precision. These findings demonstrate that we reference touch to a representation of the body that is categorically segmented into discrete parts, which consequently influences the perception of tactile distance.
  • Cognition.Cognition.2014 May;131(2):254-62. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.01.005. Epub 2014 Feb 19.
  • The tactile surface forms a continuous sheet covering the body. And yet, the perceived distance between two touches varies across stimulation sites. Perceived tactile distance is larger when stimuli cross over the wrist, compared to when both fall on either the hand or the forearm. This effect could
  • PMID 24561189
  • The putative pigeon homologue to song bird LMAN does not modulate behavioral variability.
  • Helduser S1, Westkott M2, Pawelzik K2, Güntürkün O3.Author information 1Department of Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Biopsychology, Ruhr-University Bochum, D-44780 Bochum, Germany. Electronic address: Sascha.Helduser@rub.de.2Department of Physics, Institute for Theoretical Physics, University Bremen, D-28359 Bremen, Germany.3Department of Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Biopsychology, Ruhr-University Bochum, D-44780 Bochum, Germany.AbstractThe active generation of behavioral variability is thought to be a pivotal element in reinforcement based learning. One example for this principle is song learning in oscine birds. Oscines possess a highly specialized set of brain areas that compose the song system. It is yet unclear how the song system evolved. One important hypothesis assumes a motor origin of the song system, i.e. the song system may have developed from motor pathways that were present in an early ancestor of extant birds. Indeed, in pigeons neural pathways are present that parallel the song system. We examined whether one component of these pathways, a forebrain area termed nidopallium intermedium medialis pars laterale (NIML), is functionally comparable to its putative homologue, the lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (LMAN) of the song system. LMAN conveys variability into the motor output during singing; a function crucial for song learning and maintenance. We tested if NIML is likewise associated with the generation of variability. We used a behavioral paradigm in which pigeons had to find hidden target areas on a touch screen to gain food rewards. Alterations in pecking variability would result in changes of performance levels in this search paradigm. We found that pharmacological inactivation of NIML did not reduce pecking variability contrasting increases of song stereotypy observed after LMAN inactivation.
  • Behavioural brain research.Behav Brain Res.2014 Apr 15;263:144-8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.01.019. Epub 2014 Jan 28.
  • The active generation of behavioral variability is thought to be a pivotal element in reinforcement based learning. One example for this principle is song learning in oscine birds. Oscines possess a highly specialized set of brain areas that compose the song system. It is yet unclear how the song sy
  • PMID 24485917

和文文献

  • ヒーリングタッチ実施前後における身体的・精神的変化 : 二重盲検法による準ランダム化比較試験
  • 山本 晴美,森田 久美子
  • 日本看護技術学会誌 14(2), 174-184, 2015-08
  • NAID 40020569175
  • A Wearable Haptic Device Based on Twisting Wire Actuators for Feedback of Tactile Pressure Information
  • Motamedi M. Reza,Florant David,Duchaine Vincent
  • Journal of robotics and mechatronics 27(4), 419-429, 2015-08
  • NAID 40020558497
  • SID2015報告 : Touch and Interactivity (電子ディスプレイ SID'15報告会)
  • 柳瀬 慈郎
  • 電子情報通信学会技術研究報告 = IEICE technical report : 信学技報 115(157), 9-12, 2015-07-28
  • NAID 40020558517
  • SID2015報告 : Automotive & Imaging (電子ディスプレイ SID'15報告会)
  • 髙﨑 直之
  • 電子情報通信学会技術研究報告 = IEICE technical report : 信学技報 115(157), 5-8, 2015-07-28
  • NAID 40020558499

関連リンク

六本木ヒルズにある最高、最良素材のタオルを中心としたバススタイルとギフトを提案するTOUCH。 ... 2013/12/26 TOUCH THANKS SALE 2013/12/26 福来たる幸せ 2013/12/26 TOUCH WEB SHOP THANKS SALE 2013/12/17 ...
圧倒的に薄く、カラフルなiPod touch。音楽やビデオの再生、ゲームやアプリケーション。ビデオ通話など楽しさがたっぷり。驚くほど美しい写真やHDビデオも撮影できます。
「24-TWENTY FOUR-」のキーファー・サザーランド主演の大人気海外ドラマの第2弾。世界を変える“奇跡の数字”を示す少年がその能力を狙われるミステリー。 ... 「24 -TWENTY FOUR-」のキーファー・サザーランドと「HEROES/ヒーローズ ...

関連画像


押しても画像が表示されない場合はサーバが混雑しています。2週間ほどあけて、再度押下してください。


★リンクテーブル★
リンク元tactile」「触覚」「palpatory」「触る」「タッチ
拡張検索toucher」「touchiness」「cartouche
関連記事touched」「touching

tactile」

  [★]

  • adj.
  • 触覚の
tactile sensationtactile sensetouch

WordNet   license wordnet

「producing a sensation of touch; "tactile qualities"; "the tactual luxury of stroking silky human hair"」
tactual

PrepTutorEJDIC   license prepejdic

「触覚の,触覚による,触知しうる」

触覚」

  [★]

tactile sensetactile sensationtouch sensationtouchtactile
触感覚
触診触れる触るタッチ微小神経電図圧覚



palpatory」

  [★]

  • adj.
  • 触診の
palpatepalpationtouch

WordNet   license wordnet

「relating to or involving palpation」

触る」

  [★]

touch
触診触覚触れるタッチ

タッチ」

  [★]

touch
触診触覚触れる触る


toucher」

  [★]

WordNet   license wordnet

「a person who causes or allows a part of the body to come in contact with someone or something」

touchiness」

  [★]

PrepTutorEJDIC   license prepejdic

「怒りっぽいこと,神経過敏 / 取り扱いにくいこと」

cartouche」

  [★]

WordNet   license wordnet

「a cartridge (usually with paper casing)」
cartouch


touched」

  [★]

WordNet   license wordnet

「having come into contact」

PrepTutorEJDIC   license prepejdic

「感動した / 《話》ちょっと頭がおかしい」

touching」

  [★]

PrepTutorEJDIC   license prepejdic

「人の心を動かす,哀れっな,いたましい / …について,関して」


"http://meddic.jp/touch" より作成


★コメント★

[メモ入力エリア]
※コメント5000文字まで
ニックネーム:
コメント:




表示
個人用ツール


  meddic.jp

リンク
連絡